Skip to main content

Genomic analysis of firework fear and noise reactivity in standard poodles

Abstract

Background

Fear of firework noises and other loud, sudden noises (noise reactivity) is a significant problem for many dogs and may have a negative effect on both welfare and, in severe cases, the life expectancy of dogs. A wide range of behavior traits, including fear-related behaviors, have high heritability estimates in dogs. The aim of this study was to estimate genomic heritability for fear of fireworks and loud noises in dogs.

Results

A genomic heritability estimate was performed based on genome-wide SNPs from standard poodles with records of fear of fireworks and noise reactivity. The study was based on questionnaires answered by owners, who also volunteered to return a cheek swab from their dog for DNA analyses. SNP-based heritability was estimated to be 0.28 for firework fear and 0.16 for noise reactivity. We also identified an interesting region on chromosome 17 that was weakly associated with both traits.

Conclusions

We have estimated low to medium genomic heritabilities for fear of fireworks and noise reactivity in standard poodles. We have also identified an interesting region on chromosome 17, which harbors genes that have been shown to be involved in different psychiatric traits with anxiety components in humans. The region was associated with both traits; however, the association was weak and need further verification from other studies.

Plain English summary

The prevalence of fear of fireworks and loud noises is very high in many dog ​​breeds and can be a serious problem for both the dogs and the owners. A genetic study of fear of fireworks and fear of other loud noises was conducted on standard poodles. The study was based on owner questionnaires, where the dog owners scored their dogs’ degree of fearfulness from 1 (not fearful) to 5 (very fearful). We estimated genomic heritabilities of 28% for fear of fireworks and 15% for noise reactivity. We also identified a region on chromosome 17 with a possible association with the two traits. This genomic region contains genes of interest in human anxiety-related disorders. Genomic studies in dog breeds where the individuals with and without fear-associated phenotypes can be clearly distinguished, based on owner records, may provide opportunities for the implementation of methods for genomic selection for those fear-associated traits. The results of this study may be helpful for standard poodle breeders in their selection of breeding animals, which ultimately will contribute to better animal welfare.

Background

Studies have shown that up to 30% of dogs of some breeds, including the standard poodle, show a strong or extreme fear of loud noises and/or fireworks [1–6]. Fearfulness and anxiety disorders constitute a large proportion of behavioral problems in both family dogs and working dogs, and noise reactivity is a large part of these problems [1–3, 5]. Noise reactivity is a complex trait with a wide spectrum of phenotypes, and is likely affected by both environmental factors, as well as heritage. The etiology of fear and anxiety is poorly understood. While fear is a natural response to potentially dangerous stimuli or situations, and is necessary for survival [7–11], excessive fear responses out of context are problematic and may be pathological [12]. Anxiety and fear can also have a strong negative effect on animal welfare [13], as well as the relationship between the owner and the dog [14, 15]. In severe cases, anxiety and fear may impact the life expectancy of the dog [16, 17].

Studies from human behavior research have estimated heritabilities for many personality and behavior traits to be between 20 and 70% [18–20]. These traits include aggressiveness, and disorders like social anxiety and major depression disorder [21–24]. Identifying specific genes associated with behavior traits has, however, proven to be difficult. Several behavior traits, including a range of fear phenotypes, have been shown to have high heritability also in dogs. For example, Goddard and Beilharz found heritability estimates of 0.46 for fearfulness in guide dogs, and Ruefenacht et al. found heritability estimated to 0.23 for reaction to gunfire, similar results have been reported by other researchers [25–28]. The relatively high heritability of behavior traits in dogs is also supported by the variation in prevalence of different behaviors between breeds [29, 30]. Despite the many studies in both humans and several other species, there is limited information about the genetic architecture of phobias, anxieties, and fearful behavior [12, 31, 32].

In recent years, hundreds of human genome-wide association studies (GWAS) have provided new information about genetic associations to disorders like general anxiety, major depressive disorder, schizophrenia, autism spectrum disorders and bipolar disorder and others, indicating that most behavior traits have a complex genetic background where many different loci may be involved [33–37]. GWAS in dogs has successfully identified candidate markers and genes for several behavior traits [38–41] including fearfulness [42–44]. Studies have found significant differences in the frequency of noise reactivity between dog breeds, which suggest this trait has a relevant genetic component [1, 29, 45].

The evolutionary bottlenecks and breeding for specific traits have caused purebred dogs to have lower genetic diversity with longer linkage disequilibrium (LD) compared to humans [46–48]. The accumulation of risk alleles associated with specific behaviors, as well as the limited genetic heterogeneity within breeds, makes the dog a good model for identifying associated loci for complex traits, even with a limited sample size.

The aim of this study was to estimate the genomic (SNP-based) heritability of fear of fireworks and loud noises in standard poodles and search for potential genomic regions associated with these traits.

Results

Heritability estimates

The genomic heritability estimates were 0.28 (SE 0.10) for firework fear and 0.16 (SE 0.10) for noise reactivity, with a high genetic correlation between the two traits, 0.99 (SE 0.05).

Genome-wide association study

Mixed linear models for the phenotypes firework fear and noise reactivity each identified suggestive association (p < 2 × 10− 05) to a region on chromosome 17 (CFA17) (Fig. 1). For firework fear the top SNP (BICF2P1194351) was found in position 44.487.783 (p = 6.065 × 10− 06) and the top SNP for noise reactivity (BICF2P966078) in position 44.409.723 (Table 1). Top SNPs for both traits are located in the same chromosomal region, see Fig. 2. The SNPs are in high level of LD with the neighboring SNPs. Both top SNPs are within the same intron of the Catenin alpha 2 gene (CTNNA2), and in < 1 Mb range of two other genes; Leucine rich repeat transmembrane neuronal 1 (LRRTM1) and Regenerating islet-derived protein III-alpha (REG3A) (Table 2). As shown in Fig. 2, the LD decays around 1.5 Mb on each side of the top SNPs, giving a region/LD block of about 3 Mb (2.973.859). The relevant genes (CTNNA2, LRRTM1, REG3A) within this region are depicted in Fig. 2. CTNNA2 and LRRTM1 are within the highest LD region and would be the most likely candidate genes (Fig. 2, Table 2). SNPs on chromosomes 7 and 15 also reach near-suggestive p-values (Fig. 1). All genomic positions are given according to the assembly GSD_1.0 (CanFam4) [50].

Fig. 1
figure 1

Manhattan plots for a firework fear and b noise reactivity. A suggestive line (blue) was placed at p = 2 × 10−05, based on Bonferroni adjustment using the number of independent haplotype blocks (Karlsson et al., 2013). The more conservative GWAS significance line (red) was placed at p = 3.54 × 10− 07 using the total number of tested SNPs (141.174 SNPs after QC). Quantile-quantile plots for c firework fear and d noise reactivity. Lambda was calculated at 1.0 for both traits

Table 1 Top SNPs on chromosome 17 with position, p-values, risk alleles and allele frequency in the case group and the control group for firework fear and noise reactivity
Fig. 2
figure 2

Chromosomal position of top associated SNPs for firework fear and noise reactivity, LD to top SNP is indicated with different colors. Interesting genes in the region are also included. The plots were created using LocusZoom-like [49]

Table 2 Genes in the nearby region of top SNPs. The distance between top SNPs of the two traits is 0.08 Mb

The Multidimensional scaling (MDS)-plot show an even spread of cases and controls for both firework fear and noise reactivity (Fig. 3) and for male and female between the clusters (supplementary Fig. 1). Clustering in the population can be partly explained by subpopulations with different solid colors, but cases and controls are evenly distributed in both subpopulations (supplementary Fig. 2).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Multidimensional scaling plot for a firework fear and b noise reactivity. NA = dogs with score 2 or 3

Discussion

Our estimates showed medium to low genomic heritabilities for fear of fireworks and noise reactivity. In a previous study, pedigree-based heritability for noise reactivity and fear of fireworks was estimated to be 0.09 (noise reactivity) and 0.16 (fear of fireworks), with high genetic correlation between the two traits (rg > 0.95) [51]. It has been argued that GCTA may overestimate the genomic heritability [52], but both the pedigree-based and genomic heritabilities are based on partly different and relatively small materials.

The genetic heterogeneity is lower within a dog breed than in humans, and it should therefore be easier to identify genes for complex traits with smaller materials within a single dog breed compared to humans. Still, our study supports the challenge of identifying genomic regions associated with behavior traits in general regardless of species. The region on CFA17 with a suggestive association to fear of fireworks and noise reactivity is just above the suggestive threshold with much “background noise”, as visualized on the Manhattan plots (Fig. 1). It is however notable that the two traits in question were both associated with the same region, and this region contains several genes with a potential effect on the studied traits. The results are not significant according to strict Bonferroni thresholds. Studies have argued that the traditional GWAS significance levels, using the number of tested SNPs, are too conservative in canine GWAS studies due to long LD and closely linked SNPs [48, 53]. Karlsson et al., [53] suggest that p-values < 2 × 10− 05 (depending on breed) may be an alternative to correct for the number of haplotype-blocks. The top SNPs passed the significance threshold when we used less strict criteria based on average LD of 1 Mb, 2400 blocks and p-values < 2 × 10− 05. The study is likely to be underpowered, and an increase in sample size would be preferable.

One of the genes in the candidate region, CTNNA2 (Catenin alpha 2) (Fig. 2), has been thought to be involved in bipolar disorder, a disease with a component of anxiety. The CTNNA2 gene encodes a catenin protein which is associated with several human psychiatric disorders with components of anxiety, including bipolar disorder [54] and schizophrenia [55]. The CTNNA2 gene is proposed as a candidate gene by Tang et al. (2014) in canine compulsive disorder in Doberman pinchers, a condition closely linked to anxiety [38, 56].

LRRTM1 (Leucine-rich repeat transmembrane neuronal 1) is a small protein coding gene with only 1568 base pairs. LRRTM1 is a nested gene within the bounds of the CTNNA2 gene. The LRRTM1 gene has previously been related to claustrophobia-like behavior in LRRTM1-deficient mice [57], and schizophrenia in humans [58–60].

Classification of noise reactive dogs is challenging, as a number of environmental factors influence the development and expression of the traits. The owners’ abilities to perform an “objective” and correct classification will vary, which may be a challenge as each dog has a different owner [61, 62]. Fear of fireworks might be present from early puppyhood but could also be a result of previous experiences in the dog’s life, like reactions to the smell of smoke, flashing lights, and other environmental factors. It has also been suggested that some noise-reactive dogs are suffering from physical pain in the ears or changes in auditory response [3, 63]. In general, however, owner-based questionnaires are considered an acceptable method for collecting behavioral phenotypes [64–67].

In a previous study [68], we found a correlation between the fireworks/loud noise scores and the number of fear-related behaviors observed by the owners. Dogs with scores 1 showed no fear-related behaviors, while dogs with scores 4 and 5 showed on average > 3 fear-related behaviors. None of the dogs with a score of 4 or 5 showed less than one fear-related behavior. The same study found that owners were consistent in their scoring of the same dog over time and that dogs with an initial score of 1 (controls) very rarely are reclassified to a score > 1.

Older poodles have been found to show stronger or more signs of fear than younger dogs [68] and we, therefore, had special attention to age in cases and controls. Approximately 40% of the fearful standard poodles showed signs of fear before they were 1 year old, the other 60% showed such signs, on average, before 3.9 years of age, with the median age of onset at 3. For this study, data and DNA-samples, were primarily collected from older dogs. This was done to obtain a relatively equal average age in cases and controls and to minimize the risk of false negatives in the control group. The average age of controls was 6.8 (fireworks) and 7.2 (noise), and the average age of cases was 8.2 for both traits. Thirteen dogs younger than 3 years, with a well characterized control-phenotypes, were included in the control group but removing these 13 controls did not affect the result, neither did including age as a covariate.

With our material, it is difficult to differentiate if fear of fireworks and noise reactivity are two distinct biological traits, but the two traits are shown to have a high correlation in standard poodles [68]. The two traits may, at least partly, be two different traits with some common risk loci. All the dogs that are described as very or extremely fearful of loud noises (cases) are also found to be very or extremely fearful of fireworks. Only about 50% of all dogs included as cases (score 4 or 5) for the fear of fireworks trait are also classified as cases in the noise reactivity trait. The majority of the dogs that are included as firework fear cases, and not included as noise reactivity cases, have a score of 2 or 3 on noise reactivity, but 13 dogs are included as firework case (score 4 or 5) and noise controls (score 1) (Fig. 4). It, therefore, seems that many of the firework cases could be reacting to the noise regardless of its origin (noise reactive), while some react exclusively or more severely to fireworks (fearful of fireworks). This could be explained if some of the reactivity to firework is caused by other stimuli, like flashing lights or the smell of gunpowder. If so, this suggest that fear of noise and fear of fireworks are, at least to some extent, two different traits, which are very hard to distinguish. That would also make it likely that many dogs are affected by both traits, reacting both to the loud noises and to other factors involved with firework fear, which complicates the phenotyping further. To distinguish between the two traits an appropriate material of dogs with fear of firework but not for noise reactivity would be needed. Unfortunately, only 13 firework cases in our material were without noise reactivity (score = 1). This was insufficient to distinguish the two traits in the analysis. We exploratively also analyzed the 82 firework cases that did not reach the inclusion criterium as noise reactivity cases, against the 219 firework controls, but in this analysis, the signal on CFA17 disappeared.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Distribution of cases (score 4 + 5) and controls (score 1) for noise reactivity (NR) and firework fear (FF) in the 400 standard poodles included in the GWAS. Dogs with scores 2 or 3 are not included

Phenotypes were collected from owner-based questionaries, where the dogs were scored from 1 (not fearful) to 5 (very fearful). To increase the likelihood of identifying associated loci, we maintained a maximum contrast between observed phenotype in cases and controls. Accordingly, dogs with a score of 2 or 3 were not included in the study (n = 111). Several recent studies [2, 42, 43] have included all dogs with scores > 1 as cases. One advantage of such classification is a significant increase in the number of dogs analyzed, but the disadvantage is a smaller contrast between cases and controls where owners’ ability to correctly classify the dogs’ behavior may be a challenge. By including all dogs with scores > 1 it is possible that the statistical power could be increased somewhat.

One study by Zapata [44] found an associated region of non-social fear on CFA18 in a study on 11 different breeds. Another study by Sarviaho (2019) [42],found an association of noise reactivity in an area on chromosome 20 in German shepherd dogs. Our study does not replicate those results in standard poodles. This could be due to different causal variants between the two dog breeds but also differences in the allele frequency and LD structure may affect the results.

Conclusion

Using material from standard poodles and owner-based records, we have estimated medium and low genomic heritabilities for fear of fireworks and noise reactivity, respectively. The significant medium genomic heritability for fear of fireworks may provide helpful information for genomic selection of breeding animals in future breeding programs, which may in turn reduce the prevalence of these behavior traits. We have also identified an interesting region on CFA17, which harbors genes that have been shown to be involved in several psychiatric traits with anxiety components in humans. As in human studies, it has been challenging to identify strongly associated loci, and further research is needed to validate the suggested associated region.

Materials and methods

Questionnaires

Data on behavior traits in Norwegian standard poodles were collected from web- and telephone-based questionnaires. The surveys were performed in collaboration with the Norwegian poodle club (NPC) and the Norwegian Kennel Club (NKC).

The first survey (2014) had a set of questions regarding behavior and fearfulness, including fear of fireworks and noise reactivity. The second and third surveys (2017) were performed as an initial telephone survey followed by an online survey repeating the questions about fear of fireworks and noise reactivity. Finally, a fourth and fifth online questionnaire (2020 and 2022) with the same questions about fear of fireworks and loud noises was sent to owners that had not participated in the earlier studies. In all five surveys, the dog owners were asked to indicate 1) their dogs’ fear of fireworks and 2) their dogs’ reactivity to loud noises, including thunder and gunshots, on a 1 to 5 Likert scale: “Does your dog show signs of fear when exposed to firework noises?” 1 = No signs, 2 = Some signs, 3 = Obvious signs, 4 = Strong signs, 5 = Very strong signs. A translation of the relevant questions (from Norwegian) is included as supplementary Table 1.

Included dogs

Dogs with a score of 1 (no fear) were included as controls, and dogs with a score of 4 or 5 (fearful, very fearful) were included as cases. The mean age of all dogs was 7.4 years. The mean age of controls was 6.8 (fireworks) and 7.2 (noise), with 6 being the median. Thirteen of the included controls were between 2 and 3 years old. The mean age of the cases was 8.2 for both traits, with a median of 7. The youngest included case was 1 year old. Some dogs had scores from more than one study. If the dog had more than one observation, and those scores deviated, the score from the newest online questionnaire was selected. An overview of the distribution of the selected cases and controls is given in Fig. 4.

DNA samples were collected using DNA Genotecℱ Performagene PG-100 buccal swabs or EDTA blood collected by a veterinarian. All materials were gathered in accordance with the Norwegian National Committee for Research Ethics in Science and Technology’s (NENT) guidelines for research ethics in science and technology (2007). Extraction of DNA was done in accordance with Performagene 0.5 mL purification protocol using the PG-100 kit (buccal swabs) or Omega Bio-tek - E.Z.N.A¼ Blood DNA Mini Kit (blood). A total of 400 dogs were genotyped with the 230 K Illumina HD Canine SNP-Array (Neogen Genomics, Lincoln, NE, USA).

Quality control

A genotyping quality control (QC) was performed. Markers with a minor allele frequency threshold (MAF) less than 0.05 and a call rate < 95% were excluded from the analyses, as well as markers failing the Hardy- Weinberg equilibrium exact test with a level of < 10− 6 in controls and < 10 − 10 in the cases. Samples with a genotyping rate below 95% and a heterozygosity rate above three standard deviations from the mean were removed. In addition, a control for duplicates and a gender check to identify potential sample mix-ups were performed. After quality control, 145,725 SNPs remained for the noise reactivity dataset and 145,723 SNPs in the firework datasets. Three hundred eighty-five dogs remained in both datasets, including 150 cases and 212 controls for the firework phenotype and 72 cases and 200 controls for the noise reactivity phenotype (categorical trait).

Heritability estimates

The SNP-based heritability was calculated in GCTA. A genetic relationship matrix (GRM) was calculated based on all the autosomal SNPs in the dataset. The GRM was included in a bivariate genomic restricted maximum likelihood (GREML) analysis to estimate the variance explained by the autosomal markers in the dataset using the model,

y = Ό + g + e, where y is a vector of one of the two phenotypes (fear of fireworks and noise reactivity), ÎŒ is the mean term (fixed effect), g is the random genetic effect, and e is the residual error [69]. The model was tested without the youngest controls (n = 13).

Genome-wide association analyses

The association analyses were performed using a mixed linear model in GCTA [69], including 145,097 autosomal markers where noise reactivity and fear of fireworks were used as dependent categorical variables, and a relationship matrix was included as a random effect to correct for relationship and population structure. Initial model testing showed limited effects of potential covariates like age, sex, coat color, and questionnaire. Manhattan plots and QQ-plots were created using R with the QQ-man package [70].

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  1. Storengen LM, Lingaas F. Noise sensitivity in 17 dog breeds: prevalence, breed risk and correlation with fear in other situations. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2015;171:152–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Salonen M, Sulkama S, Mikkola S, Puurunen J, Hakanen E, Tiira K, et al. Prevalence, comorbidity, and breed differences in canine anxiety in 13,700 Finnish pet dogs. Sci Rep. 2020;10:2962.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Tiira K, Sulkama S, Lohi H. Prevalence, comorbidity, and behavioral variation in canine anxiety. J Vet Behav-Clin Appl Res. 2016;16:36–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Sherman BL, Mills DS. Canine anxieties and phobias: an update on separation anxiety and noise aversions. Vet Clin North Am Small Anim Pract. 2008;38:1081–106, vii.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Blackwell EJ, Bradshaw JWS, Casey RA. Fear responses to noises in domestic dogs: prevalence, risk factors and co-occurrence with other fear related behaviour. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2013;145:15–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Dale AR, Walker JK, Farnworth MJ, Morrissey SV, Waran NK. A survey of owners’ perceptions of fear of fireworks in a sample of dogs and cats in New Zealand. NZ Vet J. 2010;58:286–91.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. McFarland D. The Oxford companion to animal behaviour, vol. viii. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1981. p. 657.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Wilson B, Serpell J, Herzog H, McGreevy P. Prevailing clusters of canine behavioural traits in historical US demand for dog breeds (1926-2005). Animals. 2018;8:197.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Gullone E. The development of normal fear: a century of research. Clin Psychol Rev. 2000;20:429–51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Erhardt A, Spoormaker VI. Translational approaches to anxiety: focus on genetics, fear extinction and brain imaging. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2013;15:417.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Ressler KJ. Translating across circuits and genetics toward progress in fear- and anxiety-related disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2020;177:214–22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Overall KL. Manual of clinical behavioral medicine for dogs and cats. St. Louis: Elsevier Mosby; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Rugbjerg H, Proschowsky HF, Ersboll AK, Lund JD. Risk factors associated with interdog aggression and shooting phobias among purebred dogs in Denmark. Prev Vet Med. 2003;58:85–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Udell MA, Dorey NR, Wynne CD. What did domestication do to dogs? A new account of dogs’ sensitivity to human actions. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc. 2010;85:327–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Shore ER. Returning a recently adopted companion animal: adopters’ reasons for and reactions to the failed adoption experience. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. 2005;8:187–98.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Dreschel NA. The effects of fear and anxiety on health and lifespan in pet dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2010;125:157–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Scarlett JM, Salman MD, New JG Jr, Kass PH. Reasons for relinquishment of companion animals in U.S. animal shelters: selected health and personal issues. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. 1999;2:41–57.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Horn JM, Plomin R, Rosenman R. Heritability of personality traits in adult male twins. Behav Genet. 1976;6:17–30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Bratko D, Butković A, Vukasovic T. Heritability of personality. Psihologijske Teme. 2017;26:1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Vukasovic T, Bratko D. Heritability of personality: a meta-analysis of behavior genetic studies. Psychol Bull. 2015;141:769–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Beatty MJ, Heisel AD, Hall AE, Levine TR, La France BH. What can we learn from the study of twins about genetic and environmental influences on interpersonal affiliation, aggressiveness, and social anxiety?: a meta-analytic study. Commun Monogr. 2002;69:1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Plomin R, Owen MJ, Mcguffin P. The genetic-basis of complex human behaviors. Science. 1994;264:1733–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Guffanti G, Gameroff MJ, Warner V, Talati A, Glatt CE, Wickramaratne P, et al. Heritability of major depressive and comorbid anxiety disorders in multi-generational families at high risk for depression. Am J Med Genet Part B: Neuropsych Genet. 2016;171:1072–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Moreno AL, Osório FL, Martín-santos R, Crippa JAS. Heritability of social anxiety disorder: a systematic review of methodological designs. Arch Clin Psych. 2016;43:83–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Persson ME, Roth LSV, Johnsson M, Wright D, Jensen P. Human-directed social behaviour in dogs shows significant heritability. Genes Brain Behav. 2015;14:337–44.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Ilska J, Haskell MJ, Blott SC, Sanchez-Molano E, Polgar Z, Lofgren SE, et al. Genetic characterization of dog personality traits. Genetics. 2017;206:1101–11.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Ruefenacht S, Gebhardt-Henrich S, Miyake T, Gaillard C. A behaviour test on German shepherd dogs: heritability of seven different traits. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2002;79:113–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Goddard ME, Beilharz RG. Genetics of traits which determine the suitability of dogs as guide-dogs for the blind. Appl Anim Ethol. 1983;9:299–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Overall KL, Dunham AE, Juarbe-Diaz SV. Phenotypic determination of noise reactivity in 3 breeds of working dogs: a cautionary tale of age, breed, behavioral assessment, and genetics. J Vet Behav-Clin Appl Res. 2016;16:113–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Storengen LM, Boge SCK, Strom SJ, Loberg G, Lingaas F. A descriptive study of 215 dogs diagnosed with separation anxiety. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2014;159:82–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Ogata N. Separation anxiety in dogs: what progress has been made in our understanding of the most common behavioral problems in dogs? J Vet Behav-Clin Appl Res. 2016;16:28–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Hohoff C. Anxiety in mice and men: a comparison. J Neural Transm. 2009;116:679–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Dick DM, Barr PB, Cho SB, Cooke ME, Kuo SI, Lewis TJ, et al. Post-GWAS in psychiatric genetics: a developmental perspective on the “other” next steps. Genes Brain Behav. 2018;17:e12447.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Horwitz T, Lam K, Chen Y, Xia Y, Liu C. A decade in psychiatric GWAS research. Mol Psychiatry. 2019;24:378–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Ripke S, O'Dushlaine C, Chambert K, Moran JL, KĂ€hler AK, Akterin S, et al. Genome-wide association analysis identifies 13 new risk loci for schizophrenia. Nat Genet. 2013;45:1150–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Ripke S, Wray NR, Lewis CM, Hamilton SP, Weissman MM, Breen G, et al. A mega-analysis of genome-wide association studies for major depressive disorder. Mol Psychiatry. 2013;18:497–511.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Wray NR, Ripke S, Mattheisen M, Trzaskowski M, Byrne EM, Abdellaoui A, et al. Genome-wide association analyses identify 44 risk variants and refine the genetic architecture of major depression. Nat Genet. 2018;50:668–81.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Tang RQ, Noh HJ, Wang DQ, Sigurdsson S, Swofford R, Perloski M, et al. Candidate genes and functional noncoding variants identified in a canine model of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Genome Biol. 2014;15:2-12.

  39. Persson ME, Wright D, Roth LSV, Batakis P, Jensen P. Genomic regions associated with interspecies communication in dogs contain genes related to human social disorders. Sci Rep. 2016;6:1-9.

  40. Shan S, Xu F, Brenig B. Genome-wide association studies reveal neurological genes for dog herding, predation, temperament, and trainability traits. Front Vet Sci. 2021;8:693290.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Gnanadesikan GE, Hare B, Snyder-Mackler N, Call J, Kaminski J, Miklósi Á, et al. Breed differences in dog cognition associated with brain-expressed genes and neurological functions. Integr Comp Biol. 2020;60:976–90.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Sarviaho R, Hakosalo O, Tiira K, Sulkama S, Salmela E, Hytonen MK, et al. Two novel genomic regions associated with fearfulness in dogs overlap human neuropsychiatric loci. Transl. Psychiatry. 2019;9:1-9.

  43. Sarviaho R, Hakosalo O, Tiira K, Sulkama S, Niskanen JE, Hytönen MK, et al. A novel genomic region on chromosome 11 associated with fearfulness in dogs. Transl Psychiatry. 2020;10:169.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Zapata I, Serpell JA, Alvarez CE. Genetic mapping of canine fear and aggression. BMC Genomics. 2016;17:572.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Morrow M, Ottobre J, Ottobre A, Neville P, St-Pierre N, Dreschel N, et al. Breed-dependent differences in the onset of fear-related avoidance behavior in puppies. J Vet Behav-Clin App Res. 2015;10:286–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Lindblad-Toh K, Wade CM, Mikkelsen TS, Karlsson EK, Jaffe DB, Kamal M, et al. Genome sequence, comparative analysis and haplotype structure of the domestic dog. Nature. 2005;438:803–19.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Chang ML, Terrill RL, Bautista MM, Carlson EJ, Dyer DJ, Overall KL, et al. Large-scale SNP genotyping with canine buccal swab DNA. J Hered. 2007;98:428–37.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Sutter NB, Eberle MA, Parker HG, Pullar BJ, Kirkness EF, Kruglyak L, et al. Extensive and breed-specific linkage disequilibrium in Canis familiaris. Genet Res. 2004;14:2388–96.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  49. Major TJ, Takei R. LocusZoom-like Plots for GWAS Results (v2.1); 2021.

    Google Scholar 

  50. Wang C, Wallerman O, Arendt M-L, Sundström E, Karlsson Å, Nordin J, et al. A novel canine reference genome resolves genomic architecture and uncovers transcript complexity. Commun Biol. 2021;4:185.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. HandegĂ„rd KW, Madsen P, Storengen LM, Lingaas F. Genetic parameters for noise reactivity in standard poodles. J Vet Behav. 2021;45:33–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Krishna Kumar S, Feldman MW, Rehkopf DH, Tuljapurkar S. Limitations of GCTA as a solution to the missing heritability problem. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2016;113:E61–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Karlsson EK, Sigurdsson S, Ivansson E, Thomas R, Elvers I, Wright J, et al. Genome-wide analyses implicate 33 loci in heritable dog osteosarcoma, including regulatory variants near CDKN2A/B. Genome Biol. 2013;14:1-16.

  54. Scott LJ, Muglia P, Kong XQ, Guan W, Flickinger M, Upmanyu R, et al. Genome-wide association and meta-analysis of bipolar disorder in individuals of European ancestry. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106:7501–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Chu TT, Liu Y. An integrated genomic analysis of gene-function correlation on schizophrenia susceptibility genes. J Hum Genet. 2010;55:285–92.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Goodwin GM. The overlap between anxiety, depression, and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2015;17:249–60.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Voikar V, Kulesskaya N, Laakso T, Lauren J, Strittmatter SM, Airaksinen MS. LRRTM1-deficient mice show a rare phenotype of avoiding small enclosures--a tentative mouse model for claustrophobia-like behaviour. Behav Brain Res. 2013;238:69–78.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Brucato N, DeLisi LE, Fisher SE, Francks C. Hypomethylation of the paternally inherited LRRTM1 promoter linked to schizophrenia. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2014;165B:555–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Francks C, Maegawa S, Lauren J, Abrahams BS, Velayos-Baeza A, Medland SE, et al. LRRTM1 on chromosome 2p12 is a maternally suppressed gene that is associated paternally with handedness and schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry. 2007;12:1129–39, 057.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Ludwig KU, Mattheisen M, Muhleisen TW, Roeske D, Schmal C, Breuer R, et al. Supporting evidence for LRRTM1 imprinting effects in schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry. 2009;14:743–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Hsu Y, Serpell JA. Development and validation of a questionnaire for measuring behavior and temperament traits in pet dogs. J Am Vet Med Assoc. 2003;223:1293–300.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Jakuba T, Polcová Z, Fedáková D, Kottferová J, Mareková J, Fejsáková M, et al. Differences in evaluation of a dog’s temperament by individual members of the same household. Society & Anim. 2013;21:582–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Scheifele PM, Sonstrom KE, Dunham AE, Overall KL. Is noise reactivity reflected in auditory response variables, including those that measure cognition, in dogs? Initial findings. J Vet Behav-Clin Appl Res. 2016;16:65–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Wiener P, Haskell MJ. Use of questionnaire-based data to assess dog personality. J Vet Behav-Clin Appl Res. 2016;16:81–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Salonen M, Mikkola S, Hakanen E, Sulkama S, Puurunen J, Lohi H. Reliability and validity of a dog personality and unwanted behavior survey. Animals. 2021;11:1-14.

  66. Tiira K, Lohi H. Reliability and validity of a questionnaire survey in canine anxiety research. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2014;155:82–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Flint HE, Coe JB, Pearl DL, Serpell JA, Niel L. Effect of training for dog fear identification on dog owner ratings of fear in familiar and unfamiliar dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2018;208:66–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. HandegĂ„rd KW, Storengen LM, Lingaas F. Noise reactivity in standard poodles and Irish soft-coated wheaten terriers. J Vet Behav. 2020;36:4–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Yang J, Lee SH, Goddard ME, Visscher PM. GCTA: a tool for genome-wide complex trait analysis. Am J Hum Genet. 2011;88:76–82.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Turner SD. Qqman: an R package for visualizing GWAS results using Q-Q and Manhattan plots. The J of Open Source Software. 3(25):731.

Download references

Acknowledgments

Thank you to the Norwegian Kennel Club and the Norwegian Poodle Club for their support in data collection.

Funding

The project was funded by the Norwegian University of Life Sciences (Veterinary faculty).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

K.W., L.M.S. and F.L. designed the study and the questionnaires. K.W. collected data through telephone surveys. K.W. and F.L. collected data from online surveys. K.W. performed the DNA extraction. K.W. and D.J. performed the GWAS analysis. D.J. estimated SNP heritability. K.W. wrote the manuscript with support from L.M.S., D.J and F.L. All authors read and approved the final manuscript

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Karin Westereng HandegÄrd.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All samples were collected as cheek swabs taken by the owner and with the owners’ consent.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: Supplementary Fig. 1

. MDS-plot showing spread of male/female dogs. Supplementary Fig. 2. MDS-plot which show that the population is split in clusters that can be explained by selective breeding on solid colors where silver, brown and fawn (red) dogs tend to be bred separately from the black and white dogs.

Additional file 2.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

HandegÄrd, K.W., Storengen, L.M., Joergensen, D. et al. Genomic analysis of firework fear and noise reactivity in standard poodles. Canine Med Genet 10, 2 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40575-023-00125-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40575-023-00125-0

Keywords